Journal of Sports Science and Medicine
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine
ISSN: 1303 - 2968   
Ios-APP Journal of Sports Science and Medicine
Androit-APP Journal of Sports Science and Medicine
Views
10660
Download
991
from September 2014
 
©Journal of Sports Science and Medicine (2005) 04, 134 - 143

Research article
Starting Block Performance in Sprinters: A Statistical Method for Identifying Discriminative Parameters of the Performance and an Analysis of the Effect of Providing Feedback over a 6-Week Period
Sylvie Fortier1, Fabien A. Basset1, , Ginette A. Mbourou2, Jérôme Favérial3, Normand Teasdale3  
Author Information
1 School of Human Kinetics and Recreation, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John’s NL, Canada
2 Unité de Recherche en Gériatrie de l’Université Laval, Hôpital Saint-Sacrement, 1050, chemin Sainte-Foy, Québec, Canada
3 Groupe de Recherche en Analyse du Mouvement et Ergonomie, Division de Kinésiologie, Département de Médecine Sociale et Préventive, Faculté de Médecine, Université Laval, Québec, Canada

Fabien A. Basset
✉ School of Human Kinetics and Recreation, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John’s NL, Canada A1C 5S7
Email: fbasset@mun.ca
Publish Date
Received: 08-11-2004
Accepted: 21-03-2005
Published (online): 01-06-2005
 
ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was twofold: (a) to examine if kinetic and kinematic parameters of the sprint start could differentiate elite from sub-elite sprinters and, (b) to investigate whether providing feedback (FB) about selected parameters could improve starting block performance of intermediate sprinters over a 6-week training period. Twelve male sprinters, assigned to an elite or a sub-elite group, participated in Experiment 1. Eight intermediate sprinters participated in Experiment 2. All athletes were required to perform three sprint starts at maximum intensity followed by a 10-m run. To detect differences between elite and sub-elite groups, comparisons were made using t-tests for independent samples. Parameters reaching a significant group difference were retained for the linear discriminant analysis (LDA). The LDA yielded four discriminative kinetic parameters. Feedback about these selected parameters was given to sprinters in Experiment 2. For this experiment, data acquisition was divided into three periods. The first six sessions were without specific FB, whereas the following six sessions were enriched by kinetic FB. Finally, athletes underwent a retention session (without FB) 4 weeks after the twelfth session. Even though differences were found in the time to front peak force, the time to rear peak force, and the front peak force in the retention session, the results of the present study showed that providing FB about selected kinetic parameters differentiating elite from sub-elite sprinters did not improve the starting block performance of intermediate sprinters.

Key words: Feedback, kinetic, kinematic, performance, sprint


           Key Points
  • The linear discriminative analysis allows the identification of starting block parameters differentiating elite from sub-elite athletes.
  • 6-week of feedback does not alter starting block performance in training context.
  • The present results failed to confirm previous studies since feedback did not improve targeted kinetic parameters of the complex motor task in real-world context.

INTRODUCTION

The sprint start is a complex motor task characterized by large forces exerted in the horizontal direction and by the ability to generate these forces in a short time period (Hafez et al., 1985; Harland et al., 1995). The starting position is an important aspect of sprint performance (Schot and Knutzen, 1992), from which the location of the center of mass (CM) and an horizontal CM velocity have been identified as descriptors of a good starting block performance (Mero, 1988). Several other kinetic and kinematic variables such as the rear peak force, the block time, the block leaving velocity and the block leaving acceleration, have been reported as possible parameters influencing starting block performance (Hafez et al., 1985; Harland et al., 1995). Most of these suggestions, however, lacked statistical support. The first part of this study aimed to use the linear discriminant analysis (LDA) to identify which kinetic and kinematic parameters differentiate most the elite from sub-elite sprinters in a starting block task. Discriminant analysis is a useful tool for detecting variables that could distinguish group differences and for classifying subjects into different groups with a better than chance accuracy. The intent of this analysis is to assess which variables are determinants of starting block performance in order to provide athletes with proper training feedback (FB).

In the early sixties, several researchers have stamped FB as being the most important variable affecting performance and learning. It was suggested that FB could increase the performance of a complex laboratory motor task and the rate of improvement on new tasks, enhance performance on overlearned tasks, and make tasks more interesting (Bilodeau and Bilodeau, 1961; Sage, 1984). Based on these assumptions, sprint coaches integrated FB into training sessions to refine athletes’ movement patterns. To date, multiple FB sessions on motor task performance have been conducted mainly on unique sessions (Harland et al., 1995; McClements et al., 1996; Mendoza and Schollhorn, 1993; Sanderson et al., 1991; Smith et al., 1997; Smith and Eason, 1990; Winstein and Schmidt, 1990), have not included skill level as a controlled variable (Kernodle and Carlton, 1992; Smith et al., 1997; Viitasalo et al., 2001; Winstein and Schmidt, 1990; Wulf et al., 1998; Wulf and Weigelt, 1997), and have not monitored the training context (environment in which the training is conducted) (Kernodle and Carlton, 1992; Smith et al., 1997; Smith and Eason, 1990; Viitasalo et al., 2001; Winstein et al., 1994; Winstein and Schmidt, 1990; Wulf et al., 1999). It would be interesting, thus, to observe in a training context whether providing FB during multiple training sessions could improve starting block performance in intermediate sprinters.

The purpose of this study was twofold: (a) to examine if kinetic and kinematic parameters of the sprint start could differentiate elite from sub-elite sprinters and, (b) to investigate whether providing feedback (FB) about selected parameters could improve the starting block performance of intermediate sprinters over a 6-week training period. We hypothesized that providing FB from performance discriminating parameters to intermediate sprinters would improve their starting block performance.

Experiment 1Methods
Subjects

Twelve male sprinters participated in Experiment 1. At the time of testing, all sprinters were in their competitive phase. Subjects gave their written informed consent, in compliance with Laval University’s Ethics Committee regulations, to participate in this experiment. Prior to the study, all subjects had achieved Athletics Canada Qualifying Standards. Based on the Track and Field Provincial Association Board criteria and on their best performance in 100 m (elite <10.70 sec; sub-elite >10.70 sec and <11.40 sec), six athletes were assigned to the elite group and six to the sub-elite group. Sprint performance and physical characteristics of the subjects are presented in Table 1.

Task, apparatus, and procedure

The data were obtained by setting the experimental starting block on the track during the 2000 Provincial Senior Track and Field Championships in Québec city. Sprinters were asked to perform three sprint starts at maximum speed using a conventional starting block and run 10 m. Hand switches started a millisecond timer and a break in the light beam of photoelectric cells (located 10 cm above the floor and 4 m from the start line) stopped the time. Perpendicular forces to the footplates were recorded by an instrumented starting block. Signals from the fixed strain gauges were conditioned and amplified (Ectron E563H, Don Mills, Ont) prior to recording at 1 kHz (12-bit A/D). The start signal was given by a gun shot and was online recorded at 1 kHz. To avoid fatigue, a 4 min rest period was given between trials. For the kinematic analysis, video records were taken at 30 frames·s-1 with six video cameras. Three cameras were placed on each side of the subject to capture the start and the first step. The environment was calibrated with a structure of known dimensions. Passive reflective markers were bilaterally placed on the skin of the subjects: feet (fifth metatarsal phalangeal joint), ankles (external malleolus), knees (lateral femoro-tibial joint), hips (greater trochanter and superior anterior iliac spine), shoulders (acromio-clavicular joint), elbows (lateral epicondyle), wrists (styloid process of ulna), and on the head (zygomatic process and glabella). A final marker was placed on the right border of the track as a reference point. Video records were software synchronized by turning on a light emitting diode that could be captured by all cameras. A voltage pulse was sent simultaneously to the A/D board to synchronize kinetic and kinematic data. All video records were captured digitally (Adobe Premiere).

Data analysis

In each frame, every marker was digitalized with software allowing determining precisely their centroid position. The 3D position and velocity of the total body CM was estimated using a 5- segment anthropometric model (foot, shank, thigh, trunk, neck and head, and arm) based on Dempster’s estimates of the segment weight and segment mass-center location (Dempster, 1955). Displacement signals were filtered (second-order low-pass Butterworth filter with a 7 Hz cutoff frequency with forward/backward passes to eliminate phase shift) and time derivatives of the linear displacements were then computed with a finite difference technique.

All kinetic parameters were analyzed using custom made software (MATLAB, MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA). Force curves were low-pass filtered in the same way as the aforementioned displacement signals. The rate of change of force production (first derivative of the force curves) was calculated to precisely identify force onsets. Figure 1 shows an example of the recorded forces. The following parameters were determined from each trial: (a) reaction time RT , defined as the time from the gun signal to the first detectable change of pressure in the instrumented blocks; (b) front force duration FFD , defined as the time between the front force onset and the front force offset; (c) rear force duration RFD , defined as the time between the rear force onset and the rear force offset; (d) total block time TBT , defined as the time between the force onset and the force offset; (e) time to front peak force TFPF , defined as the time between the force onset and the front peak force; (f) time to rear peak force TRPF , defined as the time between the force onset and the rear peak force; (g) front peak force FPF , defined as the maximal front force value; (h) rear peak force RPF , defined as the maximal rear force value; (i) delay between rear and front force onset DRF onset , defined as the onset time delay between both forces, and (j) delay between end of rear and front force offset DRF offset , defined as the time between the front force offset and the rear force offset. The 4 m run time was also recorded.

Statistical analysis

To detect differences between groups, comparisons were made using t-tests for independent samples. Variables yielding a significant group difference were retained for the LDA, which was performed to determine whether elite and sub-elite athletes differed with regard to the mean of variables entered into the model. Statistical analyses were performed using Statistica 5.5 (Statsoft Inc., Tulsa, OK).Results are expressed as mean and standard deviation (±SD). Significant difference was set at p < 0.05.

RESULTS

Table 2 shows mean values and SD of kinetic and kinematic parameters for both groups. For each subject, the three starting block trials were averaged. The t-test analysis showed that ten kinetic and kinematic parameters yielded a significant group difference. All ten variables were included in the LDA to determine to which group each observation most likely belonged. The forward stepwise LDA reduced the model to the following four variables: (1) delay between end of rear and front force offset (DRF offset), (2) rear peak force (RPF), (3) total block time (TBT), and (4) time to rear peak force (TRPF). Then, the following discriminant functions were obtained:

The LDA classification functions showed that the elite group presented the best classification (100%) whereas the sub-elite group presented three individuals erroneously classified as elite (83%).

The total LDA classification reached 92% which is considered as an acceptable value. Lambda values represent the unique contribution of the respective variable to the discriminatory power of the model. The latter showed that the DRF offset was the most discriminant variable (λ=0.664, F(1,25)=12.677, p=0.001), followed by the RPF (λ=0.495, F(2,24)=12.233, p=0.0002), the TBT (λ=0.442, F(3,23)=9.661, p=0.0002), and finally the TRPF (λ=0.296, F(4,22)=13.077, p=0.0001). Overall, this suggests that the LDA allowed differentiation of the elite group from the sub-elite group. The DRF offset was the main determinant of starting block performance among the 10 selected parameters for the sprinter sample and could be considered as a good indicator of sprint start performance since it directly affects the TBT, which was previously identified as an important starting block factor.

Experiment 2

The purpose of Experiment 2 was to examine whether providing specific FB about the identified parameters of Experiment 1 (delay between end of rear and front force offset, DRF offset; rear peak force, RPF; total block time, TBT; and time to rear peak force, TRPF) could enhance the performance of intermediate sprinters. Our hypothesis was that providing FB in a field situation would help intermediate sprinters to improve their starting block performance and consequently their 4 m run time.

METHODS

Subjects

Eight intermediate sprinters (4 males and 4 females) participated in the study, none of which had been included in Experiment 1. All subjects gave their written informed consent, in compliance with Laval University’s Ethics Committee regulations. They were active athletes from a local track and field club and had been running either at the provincial or national level for a period ranging from 2 to 6 years. All the subjects maintained their habitual training and competition schedule throughout the study, which took place during the University indoor track and field season. Sprint performance and physical characteristics of these subjects are presented in Table 3.

Task, apparatus, and procedure

The task and apparatus were identical to that used in Experiment 1 as well as the collection of kinetics variables. Since no kinematic parameter arose from the LDA model (Experiment 1), only kinetic parameters were collected for Experiment 2. Subjects were tested once a week for 12 consecutive weeks. Data were acquired during the physical preparation and the competitive periods (see Figure 2 in Appendix). Six control sessions without kinetic FB (N-FB) were conducted during the specific preparatory phase (Figure 2). During this period, the subjects still received instructions provided by their coach. The following six sessions, corresponding to the competition phase, were enriched by kinetic FB (three starts, once a week). Subjects were allowed to examine on a computer screen the force-time curves just exerted in the starting block. Prior to the first kinetic FB session, subjects were given a theoretical session about kinetic measurements in the starting block. For all FB sessions, subjects were encouraged to use visual information from the computer screen to; reduce the delay between end of rear and front force offset (DRF offset), to increase the rear peak force (RPF), and to reduce the total block time (TBT), representing discriminant variables obtained from Experiment 1. An experimenter helped the subjects to interpret the signals in order to make sure they understood perfectly the FB. Finally, all subjects underwent a retention session (without FB) 4 weeks after the twelfth session.

Statistical analysis

The statistical analysis included N-FB sessions (from the 1sh to the 6th session), the FB sessions (from the 7th to the 12th session), and the retention (the 13th session). A one-way analysis of variance with repeated measures on the factor session (13 sessions) was used to detect differences in kinetic parameters. Significant F - values were followed by a post-hoc comparison using Tukey’s HSD test. Moreover, simple linear regression analysis was used to determine relationship between strength training density and forces applied on the blocks. This was performed to ensure that block force improvements were not due to strength training (see Figure 2 in Appendix). Statistical analysis was performed with Statistica 5.5 (Statsoft Inc., Tulsa, OK). Results are expressed as mean and standard deviation (±SD). Significant difference was set at p < 0.05.

RESULTS

Table 4 presents means for the N-FB sessions (1 and 6), FB sessions (7 and 12), and R (retention) session. For all kinetic variables, no difference was observed between N-FB sessions and FB sessions and no significant improvement was observed for the 4-m run time. On the other hand, three of these kinetic variables differed significantly between the R session and the other sessions. We observed in retention: (a) a decreased in TFPF (p < 0.05) (b) a decreased in TRPF (p < 0.05), and (c) an improvement in FPF (p < 0.05). No significant relationship was found between strength training density and forces applied on starting blocks.

DISCUSSION

In Experiment 1, the linear discriminant analysis (LDA) allowed the identification of four kinetic parameters differentiating elite from sub-elite sprinters. These parameters were responsible for the difference in starting block performance and of the overall sprint performance (as defined by the sprinters’ personal best time). In spite of a small sample size, the group differences were better than chance accuracy because Lambda values were relatively high. To the best of our knowledge, it is the first time that an approach using LDA has been used to identify parameters that could explain starting block performance. Although statistical tools used in this study are mainly descriptive, they highlighted differences between elite and sub-elite sprinters. The delay between the end of rear and front force offset (DRF offset) was the main determinant of the starting block performance among the 10 selected parameters for the sprinter sample. It is surprising that this variable has never been considered as a good indicator of sprint start performance since it directly affects the total block time (TBT), which was previously identified as an important starting block factor. Harland and Steele (1997) showed that skilled sprinters exhibited shorter TBT compared to their less skilled counterparts. Moreover, the elite athletes of our study exhibited a smaller force difference between the rear and the front leg than the sub-elite sprinters (16% vs. 46%). This suggests that faster sprinters optimized their force production on the blocks. Although results of Experiment 1 showed that elite as well as sub-elite sprinters reached higher front peak force (FPF) than rear peak force (RPF), the former always displayed higher RPF than the latter, confirming Harland and Steele’s report (1997). Other authors also have observed higher RPF than FPF in skilled sprinters (Guissard and Duchateau, 1990; Harland et al., 1995; Natta and Breniere, 1998). This certainly explains why a group difference was observed for RPF and the time to rear peak force (TRPF). These results suggest that better sprinters have developed specific motor patterns adapted to the sprint task and consequently have developed a greater rate of force development (explosiveness) than their counterparts allowing a better performance.

The purpose of Experiment 2 was to examine whether providing FB over a 6-week period could enhance the performance of intermediate sprinters. Our hypothesis was that providing FB in a field situation would help intermediate sprinters to improve their starting block performance and consequently their 4 m run time. The main finding of this experiment demonstrated that 6 sessions with FB did not modify any of the variables measured. Interestingly, three variables showed an improvement but at the retention session only (shorter TRPF and TFPF as well as greater FPF).Despite these improvements, however, the 4 m run time remained constant.

Many authors have reported a positive FB effect on the learning of a complex task (McClements et al., 1996; Mendoza and Schollhorn, 1993; Sanderson et al., 1991; Smith et al., 1997; Vickers et al., 1999; Winstein and Schmidt, 1990; Wulf et al., 1998). Others, however, noted that practice variables enhancing simple skills acquisition did not seem to be efficient for complex skills gain (Wulf et al., 1999). Also, it has been suggested that observational learning is sometimes sufficient to allow the development of an error detection mechanism necessary for improving performance (Blandin and Proteau, 2000). In our experiment, the subjects were taught to use FB (i.e., specific instructions) to gain control over their response patterns. The improved kinetic parameters in retention were not related to the provided FB except for the TRPF, which was the last discriminant factor entered in the LDA model. Nevertheless, the subjects reduced their TRPF (40%) and their TFPF (24%) in accordance with an increase in FPF, RPF, FFD, and RFD of 14%, 10%, 4%, 5%, respectively (Table 4 ). This reduction in the time to peak forces with the improvement in peak forces might have increased the rate of forces development, meaning that the shape of the force curves have changed from leptokurtic curves to positively skewed curves without affecting the TBT.

It has been suggested that the effectiveness of a FB training program should be measured not by the performance during training or at the end of a training session, but rather, by the performance in a no-feedback retention session in real-world settings that are the target of training (Salmoni et al., 1984). Studies including a sport task were mainly conducted in laboratory settings raising questions about their external validity (i.e., transfer to training contexts) (Gauthier, 1985; Smith et al., 1997; Smith and Loschner, 2002; Viitasalo et al., 2001). Caution was made in Experiment 2 to provide FB in training context over several weeks when coaches were very attentive to technical aspects of the sprint start and to include a retention test one month after the last FB session. In spite of these efforts, the neutral effect of FB on starting block performance in our experiment may have been caused by the quality, quantity, and/or complexity of the provided FB. This statement is in agreement with Wulf et al. (1999), Wulf and Weigelt (1997), and Viitasalo et al. (2001) who reported that the effect of FB on a complex task might not be very effective. Compared to typical laboratory tasks, sport skills are generally more complex movements, involve the control of a greater number of degrees of freedom, require more practice to master, and take place in a specific context (Hebert et al., 1996). The starting block task was, perhaps, too complex motor a task to be modified in 6 weeks.

Finally, a simple linear regression analysis was computed to look at the strength training effect of force production on blocks. Since no significant relationship was revealed, it sounds rational to attribute peak force increases to the provided FB. Moreover, as displayed in Figure 2, the strength training density was reduced during FB sessions reinforcing the aforementioned result. Nevertheless, the subjects underwent plyometric training sessions during this phase, which had perhaps positively influenced the rate of force development.

Conclusions

In the first experiment, the LDA technique allowed identification of four kinetic parameters differentiating elite from sub-elite sprinters: (1) delay between end of rear and front force offset (DRF offset), (2) rear peak force (RPF), (3) total block time (TBT), and (4) time to rear peak force (TRPF). Experiment 2 examined whether providing FB on these variables to intermediate athletes could improve their starting block performance. Contrary to our hypothesis, FB did not help intermediate athletes to improve their starting block performance. A 6-week period is maybe too short to significantly modify performance on a complex motor task such as starting block.

AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY

Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Sylvie Fortier
Employment: Research Assistant, School of Human Kinetics and Recreation, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St-John’s, NL A1C 5S7, Canada.
Degree: BSc
Research interests: Sports Performance and Motor Learning.
E-mail: sfortier@mun.ca
 

Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Fabien A. Basset
Employment: Assistant Professor, School of Human Kinetics and Recreation, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St-John’s, NL A1C 5S7, Canada.
Degree: PhD
Research interests: Exercise physiology, sports performance.
E-mail: fbasset@mun.ca
 

Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Ginette A. Mbourou
Employment: Reasearch Assistant, Unité de Recherche en Gériatrie de l’Université Laval, Hopital Saint-Sacrement, 1050, chemin Sainte-Foy, Québec, Qc G1S 4L8, Canada.
Degree: PhD
Research interests: Motor control and gait.
E-mail: ginette.azizah@cha.quebec.qc.ca
 

Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Jérôme Favérial
Employment: National sprint coach.
Degree: BSc
Research interests:
E-mail: favj13@hotmail.com
 

Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Normand Teasdale
Employment: Professor, Groupe de Recherche en Analyse du Mouvement et Ergonomie, Division de Kinésiologie, Département de Médecine Sociale et Préventive, Faculté de Médecine, Université Laval, Québec, Qc G1K 7P4, Canada.
Degree: PhD
Research interests: Motor control.
E-mail: normand.teasdale@kin.msp.ulaval.ca
 
 
REFERENCES
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Basset F.A., Chouinard R., Lehénaff D., Fleurance P. (2002) La charge de travail en sport de haut niveau. Intégration de l’échelle de perception de l’effort dans le processus de contrôle de la charge d’entraînement : le cas de la course de demi-fond / fond. Paris. Les Editions de l’INSEP.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Bilodeau E.A., Bilodeau I.M. (1961) Motor skills learning. Annual Review of Psychology 12, 243-280.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Blandin Y., Proteau L. (2000) On the cognitive basis of observational learning: Development of mechanisms for the detection and correction of errors. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 53A, 846-867.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Dempster W.T. (1955) Space requirements for the seated operator. WADC Technical Report. Wright-Patterson Air Force Base. 55-159.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Gauthier G.M. (1985) Visually and acoustically augmented performance feedback as an aid in motor control learning: a study of selected components of the rowing action. Journal of Sports Sciences 3, 3-25.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Guissard N., Duchateau J. (1990) Electromyography of the sprint start. Journal of Human Movement Studies 18, 97-106.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Hafez A.M., Roberts E.M., Seireg A.A., Winter D.A., Norman R.W., Wells R.P., Hayes K.C., Patla A.E. (1985) Biomechanics. Force and velocity during front foot contact in the sprint start. Champaign, IL. Human Kinetics.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Harland M.J., Andrews M.H., Steele J.R., Bauer T. Ontario. (1995) XIII International Symposium for Biomechanics in Sport. Instrumented start blocks: A quantitative coaching aid.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Harland M.J., Steele J.R. (1997) Biomechanics of the sprint start. Sports Medicine 23, 11-20.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Hebert E.P., Landin D., Solmon M.A. (1996) Practice schedule effects on the performance and learning of low- and high-skilled students: an applied study. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 67, 52-58.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Kernodle M.W., Carlton L.G. (1992) Information feedback and the learning multiple-degree-of-freedom activities. Journal of Motor Behavior 24, 187-196.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Kugler P.N., Turvey M.T. (1987) Information, natural law, and the self-assembly of rhythmic movement. Hillsdale, Erlbaum.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine McClements J.D., Sanderson L.K., Gander B.E. (1996) Using immediate kinetic and kinematic feedback measured by the Saskatchewan Sprint Start System to improve sprinting performance. New Studies in Athletics 11, 137-140.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Mendoza L., Schollhorn W. (1993) Training of the sprint start technique with biomechanical feedback. Journal of Sports Sciences 11, 25-29.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Mero A. (1988) Force-time characteristics and running velocity of male sprinters during the acceleration phase of sprinting. Research Quarterly For Exercise and Sport 59, 94-98.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Natta F., Breniere Y. (1998) Influence de la posture initiale sur la dynamique du départ de sprint en starting-blocks. Science et Motricité 34, 44-51.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Sage G.H. (1984) Motor learning and control - A neuropsychological approach. Dubuque. WM. C. Brown.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Salmoni A.W., Schmidt R.A., Walter C.B. (1984) Knowledge of results and motor learning: a review and critical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin 95, 355-386.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Sanderson L.K., McClements J.D., Gander R.E. (1991) Development of apparatus to provide immediate accurate feedback to sprinters in the normal training environment. New Studies in Athletics 6, 33-41.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Schot P.K., Knutzen K.M. (1992) A biomechanical analysis of four sprint start positions. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 63, 137-147.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Smith P.J., Taylor S.J., Withers K. (1997) Applying bandwidth feedback scheduling to a golf shot. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 68, 215-221.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Smith R.M., Loschner C. (2002) Biomechanics feedback for rowing. Journal of Sports Sciences 20, 783-791.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Smith T.L., Eason R.L. (1990) Effects of verbal and visual cues on performance of a complex ballistic task. Perceptual and Motor Skills 70, 1163-1168.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Vickers J.N., Livingston L.F., Umeris-Bohnert S., Holden D. (1999) Decision training: the effects of complex instruction, variable practice and reduced delayed feedback on the acquisition and transfer of a motor skill. Journal of Sports Sciences 17, 357-367.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Viitasalo J.T., Era P., Konttinen N., Mononen H., Mononen K., Norvapalo K. (2001) Effects of 12-week shooting training and mode of feedback on shooting scores among novice shooters. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Sports 11, 362-368.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Winstein C.J., Pohl P.S., Lewthwaite R. (1994) Effects of physical guidance and knowledge of results on motor learning: support for the guidance hypothesis. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 65, 316-323.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Winstein C.J., Schmidt R.A. (1990) Reduced frequency of knowledge of results enhances motor skill learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology 16, 677-691.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Wulf G., Horger M., Shea C.H. (1999) Benefits of blocked over serial feedback on complex motor skill learning. Journal of Motor Behavior 31, 95-103.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Wulf G., Shea C.H., Matschiner S. (1998) Frequent feedback enhances complex motor skill leaning. Journal of Motor Behavior 30, 180-192.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine Wulf G., Weigelt C. (1997) Instructions about physical principles in learning a complex motor skill: to tell or not to tell. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 68, 362-367.
 
 
 
Home Issues About Authors
Contact Current Editorial board Authors instructions
Email alerts In Press Mission For Reviewers
Archive Scope
Supplements Statistics
Most Read Articles
  Most Cited Articles
 
  
 
JSSM | Copyright 2001-2024 | All rights reserved. | LEGAL NOTICES | Publisher

It is forbidden the total or partial reproduction of this web site and the published materials, the treatment of its database, any kind of transition and for any means, either electronic, mechanic or other methods, without the previous written permission of the JSSM.

This work is licensed under a Creative Commons License Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.